Transcript:
Q: Can you talk about our origins in Africa?
A: The genetic evidence suggests that we evolved in Africa.
We know that people reached Australia by forty thousand years ago,
maybe earlier, which required travelling across sixty miles of open
ocean, and it would have required a species with considerable
intelligence to put together some kind of canoe or dugout that could
have traversed that distance.
Also, probably the longer we look the more we'll find evidence for
signs of human creativity and ingenuity in Africa. Europe is where
you have a lot of caves, which preserve stuff, and Europe is where
you have a lot of archaeologists out looking for human remains, and
so I think there's a bit of a bias toward the European landscape. As
people get cleverer about finding things in Africa and look longer, I
suspect that we will see things beyond the age at which the European
artifacts appear.
We also know that a lot of our evolution had to have taken place
before the human races diverge because we're pretty much birds of a
feather. If you took a bunch of human babies from anywhere around
the world -- from Australia, New Guinea, Africa, Europe -- and scrambled
the babies at birth and brought them up in any society, they'd all be
able to learn the same languages, learn how to count, learn how to use
computers, learn how to make and use tools. It suggests that the
distinctively human parts of our intelligence were in place before our
ancestors split off into the different continents.
So in a sense we're all Africans, and if the first group that budded
off from the African population and ended up in Australia did so sixty
or seventy thousand years ago, then our evolution had to have been
pretty much complete by then, because today's Australians and today's
Europeans and today's Asians and Africans are all the same species with
pretty much indistinguishable cognitive abilities.
Q: So what happened fifty thousand years ago?
A: Human evolution, at first, seems extraordinary. How could
the process that gave rise to slugs and oak trees and fish produce a
creature that can fly to the moon and invent the Internet and cross the
ocean in boats? Was it some kind of divine spark that made our brains
special? Well, I don't think so, because I think that you can understand
human evolution in terms of the ordinary process of Darwinian natural
selection.
The way to understand how different species evolved is to think about
the niches that they fill in an ecosystem -- basically, how they make
a living. And how do humans make a living? Well, with their brains. You
could think of an ecosystem as a bunch of antagonistic arms races, almost:
Everything that an animal depends upon for food is the body part of some
other animal or plant who would just as soon keep that body part for
itself.
And so all the things that we depend on for food evolve defenses
against being eaten. Animals run away, they develop spines or poisons.
Plants can't very well defend themselves by their behavior, so they
resort to chemical warfare, and plants are saturated with toxins and
irritants to deter creatures like us who want to eat them. Now, whenever
you have some kind of defensive weapon in nature, you get an offensive
weapon, and vice versa. So as the hide gets thicker, the fangs get
stronger and sharper, which makes the hides get thicker still, and so
on.
This arms race, though, is played out in evolutionary time, and the
animal can't will its skin to get thicker in its own lifetime. Now, here's
the trick, I think, behind humans: We participate in this arms race --
but in our own lifetime, not in evolutionary time -- by using our brains,
by developing a model of how the world works, what causes lead to what
effects, and figuring out ways of defeating the defenses of other plants
and animals before they can evolve countermeasures in response.
So we invent snares or camouflaged pits, or we coordinate our behavior
to drive large animals and stampede them over a cliff, or ways of detoxifying
plants by cooking them or fermenting them or soaking them. And because
we can figure these things out in our mind's eye by learning how the
world works, we can figure out how to use more of the ecosystem to our
advantage, and I think that explains why these big-brained creatures
became as successful over the planet as they did.
Q: How did evolution, for humans, happen so quickly? We [already]
had a big brain, but how did the big brain suddenly start working?
A: Certainly humans didn't evolve to their present state in one
instant, in one fell swoop, because we know that our ancestors, the species
like Homo erectus and Homo habilis already had a pretty big
brain for a primate of that size. They were already using tools. They were
almost certainly cooperating with one another. So it's not as if our
species was the first to do it; it was building on some earlier stepping
stones.
And it's unlikely that it happened all at once. You have to remember
that not every creature that was evolving left behind its skull or its
tools for our convenience tens of thousands of years later. Most bones or
most tools rot or get buried and are never found again. So the earliest
date at which we find some fossil or artifact is not the point at which
the species first appeared; it was probably doing its thing for many
tens of thousands of years before we were lucky enough to find something
that it left behind that lasted to the present day.
Q: Can you talk about the rewiring of the human brain?
A: You have to remember that human intelligence and intelligent
behavior don't just come from having a whole bunch of stuff packed
into our skull like meatloaf. The actual organization of behavior goes
on the level of the individual nerve cells and their connections, and we
have a hundred billion nerve cells, probably a hundred trillion
connections. It's just mind-boggling to think of all the different ways
in which they're arranged in a baby's head. And a lot of our evolution
consisted not just in getting more of this stuff, but in wiring it in
precise ways to support intelligence.
Q: Does Darwinian evolution allow for such internal rewiring as
part of its process?
A: There are lots of ways in which Darwinian natural selection
could rewire a brain. There are chemicals that are released in the
growing brain that attract nerve cells, encouraging them to grow in
certain pathways versus others. There are molecules at the tips of the
growing neurons that can engage or not engage some target, like a lock
and a key. There are rules for when brain cells die in what part of the
brain, so that they might grow in one part, die off in another. All of
these are under the control of genes, and as genes evolve, the way they
do throughout evolution, the wiring of the brain can change.
Q: So this rewiring pattern happened progressively?
A: Yes. It's very likely that the changes in the brain didn't
happen overnight. There wasn't one magical mutation that miraculously
allowed us to speak and to walk upright and to cooperate with one another
and to figure out how the world works; evolution doesn't work that way.
It would be staggeringly improbable for one mutation to do all that.
Chances are there were lots and lots of mutations over a span of tens,
maybe even hundreds of thousands of years, that fine-tuned and sculpted
the brain to give it all the magnificent powers that it has today.
I don't think there was a thunderclap or a divine spark that suddenly
made one species smart. You can see, in our ancestors, there was a gradual
expansion of the brain, there was an expansion of the complexity of tools.
Even when our species evolved, it surely was spread out over tens of
thousands of years. The fact that we find a whole bunch of artwork or
tools in one place just means that that's when they arrived there and
left some garbage that survived to the present time. But it's virtually
certain that it was extended over many, many generations before that.
Q: What is a "cognitive niche"?
A: Our niche in nature, the "cognitive niche," the ability to
understand the world well enough to figure out ways of manipulating it
to outsmart other plants and animals. And there's several things that
I think evolved at the same time to support this way of life. One of
them is cause-and-effect intelligence: How do sticks break, how do rocks
roll, how do things fly through the air? A second is social intelligence:
How do I coordinate my behavior with other people so that we can bring
about effects that one person acting alone, like Robinson Crusoe, could
never have done? And I think the third is language: If I learn something,
I don't get the benefit of it alone, but I can share it with my friends
and relatives, I can exchange it for other kinds of commodities, I can
negotiate deals, I can gossip to make sure that I don't get exploited.
So, each one of these abilities -- intelligence about the world,
social intelligence, and language -- I think reinforces the other two,
and it's very likely that the three of them coevolved like a ratchet,
each one setting the stage for the other two to be incremented a bit.
Q: Some scientists think that gossip was the only thing driving
language.
A: Gossip is certainly one of the things that language is
useful for, because it's always handy to know who needs a favor, who
can offer a favor, who's available, who's under the protection of a
jealous spouse. And being the first to get a piece of gossip is like
engaging in insider trading: You can capitalize on an opportunity before
anyone else can.
But language is useful for other things, for exchanging technical
know-how -- how do you get poison out of the gland of a toad, what's
the best way to make a spear, where are the berries, what's the best
time of year to hunt. It's also good for one-on-one negotiations: "If
you give me some of your meat, I'll give you some of my fruit"; "You
and I can gang up on the leader: -- even though he's stronger than
either of us, he can't beat the two of us acting together"; "If you
have sex with me, I'll help bring up the children." There are all
kinds of ways that language can be useful. Gossip, I think, is just
one of them.
Q: So languages began just about fifty or sixty thousand years
ago.
A: We really don't know when language began. It can't be any
later than fifty or sixty thousand years ago because that's when the
races diverged, and we know that all the races are interchangeable
in their language abilities. Bring up an Australian Aborigine in New
York, they'll speak English with a New York accent, or vice versa.
So it had to be in place before that; it couldn't be later than fifty
or sixty thousand years ago.
How much earlier? I think considerably earlier, simply because
language is complicated. It's like the eyeball or the ear, and
complicated organs can't evolve in one fell swoop -- they need too
many mutations in order to craft this finely engineered organ. So I
think language had to have had a fairly long evolutionary history.
We don't really know why it took us as long to evolve as we did,
but I think there's a strong suggestion that language couldn't have
evolved before other things were in place. First of all, you have to
have something worth saying. What's the use of having long, flowery
sentences if you have nothing interesting to communicate? If chickens
had language, what would they talk about? Nothing terribly interesting.
And also, you've got to be on speaking terms with someone else. If
no one else is interested in what you have to say, or if you tell
someone something and they will take advantage of you and you can't
expect something in return, there'd be no point in having language.
So I think we evolved language when we also evolved something to say
and when we also evolved to be on speaking terms with one another.
Language evolved over an extended period of time, but it seems to
have coevolved with other things that all came to their present
configuration about the same time, somewhere before fifty thousand
years ago. Our intelligence, our language, our social interactions,
all of them seem to come together at this magic point.
I think human evolution couldn't just have been driven by social
completion, by people gossiping and plotting against each other, because
that's the equivalent of taking in one another's laundry; it doesn't
get you anywhere. I think social intelligence coevolved with physical
intelligence -- figuring out how the world works. It gives you a reason
to hang out together because you can accomplish things that one person
couldn't, and it creates an environment in which know-how is that much
more worth having because you can share it with your buddies and your
kids. And so the costs of a big brain are repaid if everything you know
can be multiplied in terms of sharing it with other people.
Q: We're talking about anatomically modern humans -- anatomically
modern and behavioral modern are two very different things. Why didn't
the others make it and why did this new group make it?
A: It's possible that once the skull had evolved to the
present size, there was still more evolving to do. And that might explain
the gap between the first anatomically modern human that had the same
amount of brain that we had, and the first behaviorally modern human who
created art and fine inventions and so on. The difference is that there
could have been a lot of evolution going on inside the skull as the brain
got rewired.
The actual cause of behavior is not just brain tissue acting en masse
like a muscle, but it's the wiring diagram of the hundred billion
different brain cells connected by a hundred trillion connections. There
are so many ways in which those could be wired and many ways for the
genes to bias that in one direction or another that, for a long period
of time, there could have been a kind of internal rewiring even if on
the outside the skull looked exactly the same.
Q: We always say that we're never going to find the answer to that
because the brain doesn't fossilize. Is that true, or do you think we
may find the answer?
A: We probably won't find the answer to that in the fossils
because the neuron-to-neuron connections certainly don't fossilize.
We'll have to be awfully clever about reconstructing it, both from the
products that they left behind -- what does a functioning brain do? --
and perhaps also from clever use of genetic evidence, working backwards
from the genes that build the brain today to figure out what the genes
that built the brain fifty thousand years ago might have looked like.
That's science fiction today, but who knows what will happen in ten or
twenty or thirty years?
Q: If you look at a Neandertal skull and the skull of the modern
human, they're about the same size. One failed and one succeeded. Why?
A: We don't really know why Homo sapiens succeeded and
Homo neandertalensis didn't. The brains were the same size, but
they may have been wired quite differently, and it could have been that
there was wiring in the Homo sapiens brain that supported better
language, cleverer know-how, better social coordination, that gave them
an advantage. And it didn't have to be a big advantage; even an advantage
of a couple of percentage points in survival rate could, over a few
thousand years, have driven the less well-adapted species to extinction.
Q: What are memes?
A: Certainly, when we look around us and are amazed at all the
things that Homo sapiens has wrought -- rockets that go to the
moon and the Internet and modern medicine and so on -- that wasn't
because our brain evolved to do those things in particular; no Robinson
Crusoe thinking by himself on a desert island could have invented a
rocket. It depends on the accumulation of an enormous number of
discoveries that were passed on, not through the genes, but from one
person to another through language and other forms of communication.
This is called cultural evolution. Some people call the units of
cultural evolution memes -- little units of memory or knowledge --
and we've been accumulating them for tens of thousands of years.
So we figured out how to make nice sharp tools and our jaws and
teeth became smaller. We figured out how to use the hides of other
animals to stay warm and we got naked. We are now figuring out how
to cure diseases, how to build shelters. And for tens of thousands
of years the products of the human brain have accumulated in almost a
parallel course in evolution to the changes in our bodies and brains.
These memes can be anything from styles that help you fit into a
group, like turning a baseball cap around and wearing the peak in the
back, to figuring out the cure for some disease or how to grow crops.
So the products of the brain that have been transmitted not through
the brains but through language have, for many thousands of years,
been as important or more important than the actual physical stuff that
we're made out of.
A lot of the creations of our brain can make up for physical
deficiencies, and could actually change the course of evolution.
Thousands of years ago, someone who was severely nearsighted probably
wouldn't have had many descendants; he would have been eaten or fallen
off a cliff a long time ago. But we invented eyeglasses and now being
nearsighted has no disadvantage at all.
There are some people who might say, "Well, isn't this interfering
with evolution? Wouldn't we be better off letting the diabetics and the
nearsighted die an early death to improve the physical vigor of the
species?" That really goes against the way that human evolution works,
which is that for tens of thousands of years we've depended for our
survival on our own inventions, on our own creation, and this is simply
extending this process.
Q: How important, in your estimation, is Darwin's theory of
evolution by natural selection to the field of biology?
A: Biologists often say that nothing in biology makes sense
except in the light of evolution, and, most importantly, Darwin's theory
of natural selection explains the appearance of design in living things.
You look at living things, and it looks as if they've been engineered.
We've got a heart that pumps blood. We've got eyes that have a transparent
lens, irises that open and close in response to the light level, and
muscles that move them in and out. We've got ears that record vibrations
of sound, and lubricated joints in our knees and elbows.
Who put them all together? Until Darwin, it would have been completely
reasonable to say, "There has to have been a cosmic engineer." For the
same reason that if we see a watch we know that there has to have been
a watchmaker, when we see an eyeball or a heart or an elbow, there has
to have been something that designed that. Darwin showed why that is
not right, that you can get the appearance of engineering in the
natural world without invoking a real engineer.
Darwin's theory of natural selection explains how we find signs of
engineering or design in the living world; why, whenever we look at a
plant or animal, we see fantastically complicated machinery.
Q: If Darwin could see the modern world, what would he be most
surprised or gratified to understand that we understand?
A: If Darwin were alive today, the discovery of biology that
would have pleased him the most would have been modern genetics and
DNA, because to the day that he died he was haunted by the worry that
his theory wouldn't work because traits of organisms blended when they
mated, that anything that was advantageous in an organism would be
diluted when it mated with some other organism that didn't also have
that trait, and there was no way to get evolution off the ground.
We know now that genes survive intact when organisms mate, that
they are particles that don't get blended but survive in their identical
form. We know that they have a physical basis, the sequence of bases in
the DNA molecule. Those were the missing pieces in the theory of
evolution, and that's really what convinced scientists that Darwin's
theory was the correct explanation for the evolution of life on earth.
Q: Do you think he would be surprised to know how much dissension
there still is around his theory?
A: I think Darwin would be surprised to learn that more than
a hundred years after he proposed his theory there are still people who
think it's just a theory, who have sincere doubts about it, because the
evidence was quite convincing in Darwin's time. And now that the last
holes of his theory have been plugged by the discoveries of genetics,
by the discovery of the age of the earth, by the discovery of the
chemical basis of life, no reasonable person can deny that this is
overwhelmingly the best explanation we have for the evolution of life
on earth.
Chimpanzees are clearly our close cousins. You cut us both open, you
see the same organs. You look at our DNA and we share 98.5 percent of our
DNA with chimps. But obviously, we're very different. Chimps are
precariously clinging to a few patches of forest in Africa; humans have
taken over the planet. What could have produced the difference?
Well, there was six million years in which our brains expanded and got
rewired in ways that allow us to do completely different things. We can
exchange information by making noise as we exhale -- the gift that we
call language. We figure out how the world works, we make many different
kinds of tools, we coordinate our behavior and exchange information. And
all of these changes in cognitive evolution, in the evolution of the
powers of the brain, account for why humans are making a film in which
they can talk about chimpanzees rather than vice versa.
A friend of mine lived and worked with a chimpanzee for several years,
and tells the story of how the chimp loved to imitate things that she did.
For example, after she washed the dishes the chimp would wash the dishes,
but the chimp's idea of washing the dishes was very different from ours.
It went through the same muscle movements; it would pick up the sponge,
let the warm water roll over his hands, would rub the sponge on the plate,
but didn't get the idea that the point of washing the dishes was to get
the dishes clean.
It just liked the feel of rubbing a sponge over the plate. It could
wash the same dish over and over again, it could rub some of the dirt
off and not get all of it off, because what it was imitating was the
particular physical sequence. What it didn't think about was what was
the goal of the human performing the action. And the ability to guess
what other people's goals are is a key part of human intelligence, and
it makes us very different from our primate cousins.
|