|
|
|
|
From Ramachandran's Notebook
Case 2
Case 1 |
Case 3 |
Case 4 |
Case 5 |
Case 6
In the mid-20th century, Canadian neurosurgeon Wilder
Penfield discovered that the entire surface of a person's
body is mapped on the surface of his or her brain. When
one touches a certain body part, say one's foot, neurons
in the part of one's brain mapped for the foot respond. A
decade ago, building on Penfield's work, Dr. Tim Pons of
the National Institutes of Health and his colleagues found
while working with monkeys that, over time, sensory
information coming from, say, the face could invade cells
of that part of the brain mapped for a dysfunctional body
part, such as a paralyzed arm. That is, the brain began
modifying the Penfield map when part of it was no longer
receiving impulses. Ramachandran wondered if this
phenomenon could explain phantom-limb syndrome. To get an
answer, he needed a human being, who, unlike a monkey,
could describe what he was feeling.
That is how I came to meet Tom [Sorenson, who lost his left
arm above the elbow in a car accident]. I called him up
right away and asked whether he would like to participate in
a study. Although initially shy and reticent in his
mannerisms, Tom soon became eager to participate in our
experiment. I was careful not to tell him what we hoped to
find, so as not to bias his responses. Even though he was
distressed by "itching" and painful sensations in his
phantom fingers, he was cheerful, apparently pleased that he
had survived the accident.
With Tom seated comfortably in my basement laboratory, I
placed a blindfold over his eyes because I didn't want him
to see where I was touching him. Then I took an ordinary
Q-tip and started stroking various parts of his body
surface, asking him to tell me where he felt the sensations.
(My graduate student, who was watching, thought I was
crazy.)
I swabbed his cheek. "What do you feel?"
"You are touching my cheek."
"Anything else?"
"Hey, you know it's funny," said Tom. "You're touching my
missing thumb, my phantom thumb."
I moved the Q-tip to his upper lip. "How about here?"
"You're touching my index finger. And my upper lip."
"Really? Are you sure?"
"Yes, I can feel it both places."
"How about here?" I stroked his lower jaw with the swab.
"That's my missing pinkie."
I soon found a complete map of Tom's phantom hand—on
his face! I realized that what I was seeing was perhaps a
direct perceptual correlate of the remapping that Tim Pons
had seen in his monkeys. For there is no other way of
explaining why touching an area so far away from the
stump—namely, the face—should generate
sensations in the phantom hand; the secret lies in the
peculiar mapping of body parts in the brain, with the face
lying right beside the hand.
I continued this procedure until I had explored Tom's entire
body surface. When I touched his chest, right shoulder,
right leg, or lower back, he felt sensations in those places
and not in the phantom. But I also found a second,
beautifully laid out "map" of his missing hand—tucked
into his left upper arm a few inches above the line of
amputation. Stroking the skin surface on this second map
also evoked precisely localized sensations on the individual
fingers: Touch here and he says, "Oh, that's my thumb," and
so on.
Case 1 |
Case 3 |
Case 4 |
Case 5 |
Case 6
Visual Mind Games
|
From Ramachandran's Notebook
|
The Electric Brain
|
Probe the Brain
Resources
|
Teacher's Guide
|
Transcript
|
Site Map
|
Secrets of the Mind Home
Search |
Site Map
|
Previously Featured
|
Schedule
|
Feedback |
Teachers |
Shop
Join Us/E-Mail
| About NOVA |
Editor's Picks
|
Watch NOVAs online
|
To print
PBS Online |
NOVA Online |
WGBH
©
| Updated October 2001
|
|
|
|