Mysteries of Sleep
Why do we sleep? And what does sleep have to do with memory, trauma, and our emotions?
From fruit flies to whales, virtually every animal sleeps. But why? Why do we need to spend nearly a third of our lives in such a defenseless state? Scientists are peering more deeply into the sleeping brain than ever before, discovering just how powerful sleep can be, playing a role in everything from memory retention and emotional regulation to removing waste from our brains. So why are we getting so little of it? (Premiered February 26, 2020)
More Ways to Watch
Mysteries of Sleep
PBS Airdate: February 26, 2020
NARRATOR: Sleep: we all do it, but why?
MATTHEW WALKER (Neuroscientist, University of California, Berkeley): Sleep remains one of those remarkable puzzles. We’ve known the functions of eating, drinking and reproducing for thousands of years. However, sleep remains a mystery.
EUS VAN SOMEREN (Neuroscientist, Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience): Why do some people ruminate all night and other people, they see the pillow, they’re gone?
NARRATOR: Cutting edge research is now giving us a new view inside the sleeping brain.
PHYLLIS C. ZEE (Neurologist, Northwestern University): How can we boost and enhance sleep quality, sleep quantity? It’s really, right now, the tip of the iceberg.
NARRATOR: If you’ve ever thought of sleep as a waste of time, think again.
RAVI ALLADA (Neurobiologist, Northwestern University): The more we learn about sleep, the more we realize we can’t dismiss it.
DAVID DINGES (Penn Medicine): Getting a good night’s sleep is possibly the single most important thing you can do every day.
NARRATOR: Mysteries of Sleep, next, on NOVA.
RAVI ALLADA: I think sleep is one of the most enduring mysteries in all of science. We spend a third of our lives asleep, in this kind of unconscious, unresponsive, immobile state. We can’t do any of the things that we think are important for our lives, like, eat, care for our young, mate.
NARRATOR: Why do we spend a third of our lives in such an unproductive and defenseless state?
ETI BEN SIMON (University of California, Berkeley): What is that thing that sleep does to our brain and our body every single night is very much an open question.
NARRATOR: A question that has baffled scientists for centuries.
YUVAL NIR (Sleep Investigator, Tel Aviv University): It’s like this big black hole. We don’t really understand why is it that we sleep and what happens in our brain when we’re asleep.
NARRATOR: But in the last decade, sleep researchers have started to unravel the mysteries of sleep, and what they are discovering is mindboggling.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: Many people think that when we sleep we are unconscious, so the brain is sort of shut off; but the more we explore it, the more it’s clear that the brain is not shut off.
GINA POE (University of California, Los Angeles): In fact, we find that the brain is just as active when we’re asleep as when we’re awake. It’s just active in different ways.
MICHEL CRAMER BORNEMANN (Lead Investigator, Sleep Forensics Associates): You’re not either aware or not aware; you’re neither not conscious or unconscious. It’s a whole spectrum.
NARRATOR: So, what exactly is sleep? And why do we need it? One thing that is for certain: when it comes to sleep, we’ve got a lot of company.
MATT WALKER: What we’re fast learning is that sleep isn’t a luxury; sleep is a biological necessity.
GINA POE: I find it fascinating, because every animal sleeps, every animal that we’ve studied, from worms to jellyfish to sea slugs. Even the octopus, whose genome is so different from our own, they sleep about as much time as we do.
ETI BEN SIMON: Sleep is one of the most essential elements of life. Actually, sleep and life evolved hand in hand.
NARRATOR: Evolution has come up with a variety of ways to get some shuteye.
JEROME SIEGEL (Neuroscientist, University of California, Los Angeles): Some animals are vulnerable when they sleep, and other animals are not. And the animals that are vulnerable when they sleep don’t sleep very much. If animals live in the open, they obviously have to be alert, and they can’t sleep as deeply. You know, if a giraffe slept the same way a lion slept, there wouldn’t be any giraffes.
Now, on the other hand, there are animals like the big brown bat, which is the champion sleeper, sleeps 20 hours a day. It sleeps on cave walls, so it’s pretty much invulnerable there.
NARRATOR: But perhaps one of nature’s most innovative sleep solutions is found under the sea.
RAVI ALLADA: One of the really cool animals that people study is the dolphin, which actually has unihemispheric sleep.
JERRY SIEGEL: Half of the brain has a sleep-like state and the other half has a wake-like state. So, the animal has to have one hemisphere awake. In fact, if you anesthetize dolphins, they stop breathing. And in the fur seals, such as the ones swimming behind us, when the right hemisphere is asleep, the left flipper, which is controlled by the right hemisphere, is inactive, and the body’s posture is asymmetric. So, by looking at a fur seal you can tell which hemisphere is asleep.
NARRATOR: Fur seals and dolphins aren’t alone. Human sleep is equally complex, weird and mysterious.
KETEMA PAUL (Neurobiologist, University of California, Los Angeles): I would say my favorite animal, in terms of how animals sleep, are humans. Human sleep is very broad. Each individual has their own personal experiences with sleep.
NARRATOR: So, what exactly is happening inside our brains when we sleep?
REBECCA SPENCER (Neuroscientist, University of Massachusetts, Amherst): We have really entered a different world once we’re asleep. I actually think that the whole night is a really magical event.
NARRATOR: With the help of volunteers like five-year-old Jaime Lopez, sleep researcher Rebecca Spencer gathers clues to how this magical event unfolds.
RESEARCHER: Slip this on, just like this.
REBECCA SPENCER: To study sleep, we equip Jaime with the sleep cap…
RESEARCHER: Shake, shake, shake, shake, shake, shake, shake.
REBECCA SPENCER: ...with an array of electrodes to record brain activity.
RESEARCHER: There you go.
YUVAL NIR: The way our brain supports everything that it does, from controlling our body to regulating emotion, having memories, is through the electrical activity of neurons.
These are brain cells connected to one another via these tiny passages that are called synapses. One neuron emits a neurochemical called a neurotransmitter to this passage, and it’s picked up by the next neuron, much like passing the baton in the Olympics.
NARRATOR: This signal, passed from neuron to neuron, can be picked up by the electrodes in Jaime’s cap, with one of the most powerful tools in a sleep researcher’s toolbox, the E.E.G., the electroencephalogram.
REBECCA SPENCER: What this screen is showing is the recordings from each of those electrodes in the cap that we put on Jaime. Right now, in wake for instance, you can see the brainwaves here.
NARRATOR: The vertical lines on the chart represent five seconds of Jaime’s sleep.
REBECCA SPENCER: What’s important is that as you get drowsy, those waves slow down and become what we call alpha waves.
YUVAL NIR: And as the sleep gets deeper, the waves become slower and slower. And in the deepest parts of sleep, activity is dominated by slow waves, these massive waves, occurring across the brain, that are like a tsunami.
MATT WALKER: It’s almost like a football stadium, where all of the individuals in the stadium before the game are all, sort of, speaking to each other at different moments, at different times. That’s what seems to happen when you’re awake. But when you go into the deepest stages of sleep, all of the sudden, the crowd starts to synchronize its activity. They all start to chant in time.
NARRATOR: Thousands of neurons, firing in unison.
REBECCA SPENCER: That’s your deep sleep. That’s when it’s hard to wake you up.
GINA POE: When you wake up someone who’s been in slow-wave sleep, and we ask them what they were thinking, they will say, “I don’t know. I wasn’t thinking anything. I was asleep. Leave me alone. Go away.” And they’ll push you off.
NARRATOR: But Jaime, along with the rest of us, doesn’t stay in deep, slow-wave sleep all night. At a certain point, his brainwaves change.
MATT WALKER: After about 50 or 60 minutes, your brain will start to rise back up. And then it will pop up and have a short REM sleep period.
Turns out that those two types of sleep, non-REM and REM, will play out in a battle for brain domination throughout the night. And that, sort of, cerebral war is going to be won and lost every 90 minutes.
NARRATOR: We spend most of the night in non-REM sleep. The rest of the time we spend in the mysterious stage of REM sleep.
ETI BEN SIMON: It’s hard to investigate REM sleep without investigating dreams, because more than 80 percent of REM periods would include a dream.
REBECCA SPENCER: Dreams tend to be emotional. One idea is that we dream to simulate potentially negative events, so that we’re prepared for them. I had a dream when my daughter was very young that she fell into the swimming pool; she was near drowning. After that, I put my daughters into swim lessons, and water safety has been important to me.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: It’s not such that we do not dream in the other sleep stages, we do, but the most vivid ones are in REM sleep.
NARRATOR: REM sleep is named for the rapid eye movements we make when we dream.
YUVAL NIR: We believe that every time the eyes move in a dream, it’s a special moment where we, sort of, switch to the next dream scene, if you will.
NARRATOR: As we switch from one dream to the next, our brainwaves are doing something downright strange.
REBECCA SPENCER: With REM sleep, the brainwaves look just like waves from when you’re awake.
PHILIP GEHRMAN (PENN Medicine): We have this paradox that it’s a state of sleep, but yet our brain is in a state of activation.
NARRATOR: But perhaps the strangest feature of this stage of sleep is what’s happening in your body.
YUVAL NIR: During REM sleep, our brain actually sends the instructions to the different muscles to move our body, as if we were awake. But lower down in the brain stem, these instructions are disrupted. They are not relayed to the body, and the body remains paralyzed. Otherwise, if we’ve have had a dream where we fly above the city, we would literally jump out of the window.
NARRATOR: From the dreams of REM sleep, we cycle back into non-REM sleep, including the deepest stage of sleep, slow-wave.
MATT WALKER: In the first half of the night, the majority of those cycles are comprised of deep non-REM sleep. Yet, as you push through to the second half of the night, now, that ratio balance shifts, and instead, the majority of those cycles are comprised of much more rapid-eye-movement sleep, dream sleep, and a lot less deep non-REM sleep.
DAVID DINGES: What is crazy about this is the pattern is so absolutely reliable in virtually everybody, every night. It speaks to a fundamental, genetically-driven program that is essential for being a human being. You have to go through this.
NARRATOR: But if every one of us needs a night full of both slow-wave and REM sleep, why is it such a struggle for so many of us to get some shuteye?
The search for answers has become a multibillion dollar industry, selling us everything from sleeping pills to ergonomic pillows. And if they don’t do the trick, there are more than 3,000 sleep clinics nationwide, a number that keeps on growing.
DREW ACKERMAN (Podcaster, Sleep With Me): (On podcast) Are you up all night, tossing, turning, mind racing, trouble getting to sleep, trouble staying asleep?
NARRATOR: Drew Ackerman has created a podcast to help the sleep deprived get their Zs.
DREW ACKERMAN: The idea for the show really sprang from my childhood insomnia. When I was a kid, I lost the ability to fall asleep. For me it was anxiety related. My parents tried to help, but because they could sleep, I think there was like this disconnect it’s like, “Oh, try to relax. Try to just think about something nice.” And I just couldn’t do that.
(On podcast) Whatever’s keeping you awake, thoughts, you know, things you’re thinking about.
NARRATOR: Drew’s goal is to be as boring as possible.
DREW ACKERMAN: (On podcast) I’m going to tell you all a bedtime story. I want you to get comfortable.
The podcast is not straightforward, it’s full of nonsense.
(On podcast) I have a unique hobby. My dog and I, we listen to recordings of people knocking on doors.
It gives people permission not to listen or to only kind of listen. I want no social pressure on the listener to pay attention to me at all.
NARRATOR: And there’s no shortage of people eager to tune out.
DREW ACKERMAN: Each month the show gets downloaded a little bit over 3,000,000 times.
(on Podcast) What I’m going to do is I’m going to send my voice across the deep dark night.
People that listen to the podcast share that feeling, I don’t know if “desperation” might be a strong word, but that feeling where you’re just lying there in bed and you feel alone.
(on Podcast) How many people listen to the podcast because they have trouble falling asleep? Like, if you want, raise your hand.
NARRATOR: Having trouble falling asleep or staying asleep are symptoms of the most common sleep disorder on the planet. Ten percent of all people suffer from chronic insomnia.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: Insomnia is a 24-hour disorder. It’s not only sleep complaints. These persons also feel tense all day. It’s not that common that you’re a very happy completely un-anxious insomniac.
LAB ASSISTANT: Are you ready?
REINY METZ (Lab Participant): Right.
NARRATOR: Is there a way to decode what goes awry in an insomniac’s brain?
On the outskirts of Amsterdam, at the Netherlands Institute for Neuroscience, Eus Van Someren is trying to find out, with the help of lifelong insomniacs like Reiny Metz.
REINY METZ: I drop off to sleep very easily, but after two hours I wake up, and then it’s difficult to go back to sleep. If it’s one night, well, you can manage that; two nights is okay. Well, when it’s five nights in a row, it’s a bit much. And then you get very, very tired. Not only physically, but also mentally. It makes me anxious or angry or, you know, frightened.
NARRATOR: Reiny sleeps in Eus’s lab, wearing a high-tech E.E.G. net that contains hundreds of more electrodes than you’ll find in the standard cap.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: We measure sleep overnight with a special E.E.G. net, with 256 E.E.G. channels, and they cover all of the head.
NARRATOR: The more electrodes he uses, the more activity he can record, more clues to what’s going on in Reiny’s brain.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: Okay, Reiny, we recorded your E.E.G. during your sleep.
NARRATOR: The next morning, he shows her the results.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: So, you go into REM sleep here. And REM sleep is the part of sleep where the most vivid dreams are. Now, what I wanted to show you is that you see that it’s not many seconds into REM sleep, and then already something is happening here. Well, you recognize it. I don’t have to explain that this looks different than this.
REINY METZ: Oh, yes, certainly.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: It’s just maybe two seconds or so that it’s, really, it’s off, it’s different. And this is what we call an “arousal,” where you exchange sleep for something that’s really wake-like.
And this is something that is so typical for people like you that sleep bad.
REINY METZ: You do see this in many patients?
EUS VAN SOMEREN: Yeah.
REINY METZ: Okay.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: Usually, if you have a good sleep, if you “slept on it,” as we say, things feel a bit better. But if there is this profile that there is some restlessness occurring during REM sleep, then, for some reason, we tried to find out, and this whole process of feeling better the next day doesn’t work as well.
What we observed, which was fascinating, if we add up all the pieces of sleep as suggested by the E.E.G., many people with insomnia have about six-and-a-half maybe seven hours of sleep, but this is not how these people experience it. Maybe they just experience large parts of the night really as ongoing rumination, worrying, thinking. So, it may not feel like a good night of sleep, but it’s not the same as being completely sleep deprived.
NARRATOR: So is there a connection between these disruptions in REM sleep and the anxiety so many insomniacs feel when they’re awake?
To try to find out, Eus comes up with an “out of the box” idea, based on personal experience. Back in the 1990s, Eus played guitar in a popular Dutch rock band.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: I remembered a few things from being in the recording studio. I heard my own guitar playing, and even if it was a tiny little bit, you know, off tune, it made me shiver.
NARRATOR: Embarrassment is a powerful emotion. He decides to put it to a test…
REINY METZ: (Singing in Dutch)
NARRATOR: …a karaoke test.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: We asked people to sing along karaoke, but they couldn’t hear themselves sing. If you don’t hear yourself well, it’s also difficult to correct if you go out of tune.
You hear that over the headphone, you have the headphone.
NARRATOR: He does the same thing with good sleepers.
Next, Eus puts them in an f.M.R.I.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: So, in the M.R.I. scanner, we had them listen to their own embarrassing singing.
REINY METZ: (Singing in Dutch
NARRATOR: As Reiny listens to her singing, the f.M.R.I detects activity in a part of the brain called the amygdala. We have two, one in each hemisphere.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: I sometimes call the amygdala the “siren” of the brain or the “alarm bell” of the brain. So, if there is something that we should pay attention to, because it’s dangerous or important, then the amygdala activates.
REINY METZ: (Singing in Dutch)
EUS VAN SOMEREN: So, they heard themselves singing really, really out of tune, and their amygdala was very upset about that, so, you know, the alarms went off.
NARRATOR: The alarms go off for both insomniacs and good sleepers. This is no surprise for Eus, but his test isn’t over yet.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: We ask them to stay all night in a sleep lab, and we did the same the next morning.
REINY METZ: (Singing in Dutch)
EUS VAN SOMEREN: We again put them in the M.R.I. scanner. For good sleepers it was not that bad anymore.
NARRATOR: A good sleeper’s amygdala calms down, but that doesn’t happen in the insomniac.
EUS VAN SOMEREN: For them, the story was very different, because the more REM sleep they had, the worse it got. So, instead of the amygdala becoming adapted overnight, for many people with insomnia, the next morning, the amygdala could even ring much louder. You’re just loaded with distress that you take to the next day and the next day and the next day.
If we could change that restless REM sleep, maybe this would help people get rid of distress.
NARRATOR: But insomniacs aren’t the only ones to suffer from restless REM sleep; researchers are also exploring its impact on post-traumatic stress disorder.
PHILIP GEHRMAN: P.T.S.D. is essentially a memory-based disorder. An individual has one or more very stressful traumatic experiences, and they become fearful of anything that reminds them of that trauma.
NARRATOR: Like the smell of a wildfire or the deafening sounds of combat, traumas so powerful they can even haunt us when we sleep.
GINA POE: People with post-traumatic stress disorder, they’re afraid to go to sleep, or they don’t sleep very well, because of the nightmares.
PHILIP GEHRMAN: Once these nightmares get established, they often can persist for decades.
NARRATOR: In her lab at U.C.L.A., Gina Poe is searching for ways to prevent these recurring nightmares from taking shape.
GINA POE: One of the things that we have to do to study the effects of trauma on sleep is we have to expose animals to a traumatic stressor. It’s the least favorite part of my job, but there’s no other way to study it.
NARRATOR: The rats are placed in a chamber where they hear a tone, followed by a shock.
GINA POE: It certainly is not a strong enough shock to cause them harm or blister their feet or anything, and it only lasts one second, but it’s enough to make them squeak and jump to say, “What? What was that?
NARRATOR: Over the next hour and a half, the rats hear the tone and receive the shock.
GINA POE: So, they associate that tone with the fear of being shocked.
NARRATOR: Then they’re taken back to their nests to get some Zs. But their sleep is anything but restful.
GINA POE: We’ve found that the REM dream state of sleep after a rat has experienced a trauma can be hyperactive. It’s kind of like REM sleep on steroids, in the way that it is in people with post-traumatic stress disorder.
NARRATOR: So, Gina comes up with a novel idea. After the shock, half the rats go right to sleep, the other half are kept awake for about six hours. In that time they eat, they play, and get a chance to calm down, before they, too, get some shuteye.
GINA POE: And the next day we bring them back into a slightly different environment. So, it smells different. It has different colors, different lighting.
NARRATOR: In fact, the only thing that remains the same, is the tone that came before the shock. When the rat that went straight to sleep hears the tone, it freezes.
GINA POE: As soon as they hear the sound, animals with P.T.S.D. will freeze.
NARRATOR: But what happens to the rat that’s been allowed to calm down before going to sleep? Will it have the same response? When it hears the tone, it also freezes, at first, but then, it seems to realize the shock isn’t coming.
GINA POE: And it will start walking around and sniffing and exploring, as rats normally do, because they realize they’re not going to be shocked here.
NARRATOR: Delaying sleep after a trauma may lessen the impact of disturbing experiences and even prevent the nightmares of P.T.S.D. from taking shape, that is, in rats.
But will it help us humans?
Gina is taking her work outside the lab, conducting a study with firefighters, asking them to delay sleep after a traumatic event.
GINA POE: With firefighters, we can ask them to do whatever it is that they do to best relax and calm themselves after trauma. For some, it might be meditation or prayer. For others, it might be listening to music that they love or going for a run. We’re going to see if that helps us prevent post-traumatic stress disorder.
PHYLLIS ZEE: I used to be asked a lot, “Why do you sleep? What’s the function of sleep?” But I think we should be asking the question, “What are the functions of sleep?”
NARRATOR: We spend about 20 percent of the night in REM sleep. The rest of the time, your brain is in non-REM sleep, part of which is spent producing those big slow waves. But, what are they for?
One of the first scientific experiments to find clues was conducted back in 1924, at Cornell University, by psychologists John Jenkins and Karl Dallenbach. Using a group of college students as guinea pigs, they found that when their students learned something new, they had a much better chance of remembering it, if they “slept on it.”
MATT WALKER: So, they found that sleep, rather than simply being a dormant state where nothing too much happens within the brain, sleep may be important for memory.
REBECCA GOMEZ (Experimental Psychologist, University of Arizona): But then it raised all sorts of questions about why, and so we’ve been trying to answer the question of why, since then.
NARRATOR: At the University of Arizona, experimental psychologist Rebecca Gomez, along with grad student Katherine Esterline, search for the “why” with the help of a younger generation of students, toddlers.
KATHERINE ESTERLINE (University of Arizona): Are you ready?
TODDLER #1: Yeah!
NARRATOR: How does sleep help them learn and remember new words?
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: Wow, a zet.
REBECCA GOMEZ: We’re teaching children novel words for a completely novel objects.
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: Look at these.
REBECCA GOMEZ: We use this completely new information, so we can measure the brain’s ability to form completely novel, completely new memories.
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: Hey, a beev.
REBECCA GOMEZ: So, in essence, we’re measuring brute force memory.
TODDLER #1: What’s this?
NARRATOR: The kids are shown four objects they’ve never seen before, that have been given nonsense names, like zet...
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: Wow, a zet. Cool.
NARRATOR: …mup...
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: A mup.
NARRATOR: …beev...
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: A beev.
NARRATOR: …and toap.
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: A toap.
TODDLER #1: Toap.
NARRATOR: They get a chance to look at the objects…
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: Look at these.
NARRATOR: …and even touch them. Then, half of the toddlers go home for their afternoon nap, while the other half don’t nap for hours or perhaps not at all.
The next day, they’re back.
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: Where’s the zet?
NARRATOR: How much do they remember?
REBECCA GOMEZ: What we found is that the children who nap soon after learning…
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: Mup?
REBECCA GOMEZ: …remember the words about 80 percent of the time.
TODDLER #1: Toap.
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: Where’s the zet?
REBECCA GOMEZ: In contrast, the kids who went through a long period of time before they napped...
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: Where’s the zet?
REBECCA GOMEZ: …only remembered the words about 30 percent of the time.
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: Where’s the toap?
TODDLER #2: This?
REBECCA GOMEZ: So you see a huge difference between 80 percent of the time and 30 percent of the time and that’s the difference the nap makes.
KATHERINE ESTERLINE: Where’s the mup?
TODDLER #3: Right here.
NARRATOR: Why did a nap make all the difference? The key lays inside a tiny organ found deep within the brain, the hippocampus. We have one in each hemisphere, and they play a critical role in helping us learn and remember.
REBECCA SPENCER: So, think of it this way. I have a little filing drawer beside my desk, and throughout the day, as papers come in, I toss them in this drawer. It’s my mail that came today; it’s some papers that I had from a class; that’s my temporary storage. The hippocampus is like that short-term filing drawer. A mishmash of information getting squeezed in, and there’s a limited amount of room there for that. But at the end of the day, I can take that information and turn around to my whole huge filing cabinet, which in this case is the cortex. The cortex, it’s bigger, and it has a really nice sorting mechanism. You can sort things by their visual components, by their auditory components. That memory becomes easier to find. So the role of slow-wave sleep is to take that information that’s been stuffed into the hippocampus and help move it to its more efficient filing system, out in the cortex.
MATT WALKER: Things that you learned yesterday are now transferred to a safer storage location. But second, when you wake up in the morning, your hippocampus has now been cleared out, and you have a refreshed capacity for new file acquisition, all over again.
NARRATOR: And that brings us back to our toddlers and the power of the midday nap. Why does it make such a big difference? Why is it so important for toddlers to clear out that short-term filing draw?
Researchers have a theory.
REBECCA GOMEZ: Young children, their brains are still developing, and, in fact, the hippocampus is still in the process of developing all across the childhood years.
REBECCA SPENCER: So, it does seem that the hippocampus, when it’s young and immature, such as in infancy and early childhood, perhaps those memories need to be stored more frequently or moved to the cortex more frequently.
NARRATOR: But that doesn’t mean naps are just for kids.
REBECCA SPENCER: It’s interesting to think that as you get older, you actually see napping start to return in a number of individuals.
NARRATOR: And there may be a good reason why.
REBECCA SPENCER: Most older adults report two frustrating things about aging: “I can’t remember things like I used to.” And “I can’t sleep like I used to.” Napping could be one way of helping maintain memories.
MATT WALKER: There are probably multiple different reasons why the aging brain simply can’t learn and remember as effectively. And I think we’re identifying that sleep is one of those critical ingredients.
REBECCA SPENCER: So, this is the sleep of an older adult. You should start seeing these waves slowing down and getting higher in amplitude. That would be what we’re looking for, for slow-wave sleep. And so far, I’m not seeing any, still looking. They’re asleep, but they’re not getting the slow wave that, in a young adult, you might expect to see by now.
MATT WALKER: And it seems to be that the quality of the sleeping brainwaves that you have, the depth of those brainwaves and the size of those brainwaves, accurately predicts how well you’re able to hit the save button on those memories.
NARRATOR: Is there a way to improve those big slow waves to increase our ability to hit that save button? Sleep researchers are exploring a radical idea.
PHYLLIS ZEE: One of the things that we’re most interested in is how can we boost and enhance sleep quality, sleep quantity by using, you know, not pharmacology, but sound?
NARRATOR: Eighty-year-old Marion Smith is participating in a sleep study. To track the quality of her slow waves, a single electrode is placed on her forehead. Marion will hear carefully timed pulses of sound through this headband, equipped with tiny speakers.
RALPH: Have a good night. I’ll see you tomorrow.
PHYLLIS ZEE: Our patient, Ms. Smith, is now clearly sleeping. She is now getting deeper sleep.
NARRATOR: By examining the brainwaves produced by a single electrode, Phyllis has all the information she needs to assess the quality of Marion’s slow waves.
PHYLLIS ZEE: These are these big slow waves, but there are very few of them. And this is quite typical of an older person who has low amplitude slow waves, and they don’t occur, like, in a train.
NARRATOR: Next, a specially designed computer algorithm measures the waves to determine the best time to deliver a particular sound, the pulsing of pink noise.
PHYLLIS ZEE: We do very brief, like, 50 milliseconds of this very short burst of pink noise. And we do it five on, five off, as long as the person is still in deep sleep.
NARRATOR: Think of the soundwaves produced by pink noise giving Marion’s brainwaves a little push, like a kid on a swing or the movement of a ball in a balance pendulum.
MATT WALKER: And what you try to do is sing in time with these brainwaves. But, by stimulating them, you’re trying to boost the size of those brainwaves.
PHYLLIS ZEE: It’s beautiful. This is what we want them all to look like, these very large, with strong upstate, waves. Even after you stopped stimulating you can see the increase in these slow waves.
NARRATOR: Phyllis is finding that a little push goes a long way.
PHYLLIS ZEE: What we’re seeing here is not only that we can increase the amplitude, that means the height of these slow waves, which is really important, but we can also increase the train. So, we could prolong the amount of slow waves, which is wonderful, because it’s hopeful that the brain, even if you’re old, is capable of boosting the slow waves.
NARRATOR: Not only might boosting slow waves improve memory, new research is revealing that during this stage of sleep the brain may be doing some critical housekeeping.
YUVAL NIR: It was discovered that the brain is actually actively flushing out cellular waste while we sleep. Just like when we visit Paris and we see them clean the streets at 5 a.m., our brain, it cleans out all the waste during this offline period.
REBECCA SPENCER: There could be a potential link between sleep and neurodegenerative diseases, like Alzheimer’s disease. And it could all come down to this brain-cleaning process that happens specifically during slow-wave sleep.
PHYLLIS ZEE: We have to do a lot more work in this area. It’s really, right now, the tip of the iceberg.
NARRATOR: While researchers explore new ways to help us get the sleep we need, millions of Americans fight the urge, staying up way past bedtime, lured by the trappings of technology.
DAVID DINGES: Nearly everyone, sooner or later, will experience some sleep loss in their life. The invasion of television into the home 50 years ago, and now computers and telephones—basically, we need to shut that stuff off.
MATT WALKER: Somewhere between infancy and even childhood, we, in western industrialized nations, we start to abandon the notion that sleep is useful, and, if anything, take the opposite approach and believe that sleep should be shortchanged.
NARRATOR: Back in 1964, 17-year-old Randy Gardner broke the Guinness World Record for staying awake 11 days straight.
JONNY OLSON (Announcer, To Tell the Truth, File Footage, May 11, 1964): One of these young men holds an unusual world record. What is your name please?
CONTESTANT #1 (Contestant, To Tell the Truth, File Footage, May 11, 1964): My name is Randy Gardner.
RANDY GARDNER (Contestant, To Tell the Truth, File Footage, May 11, 1964): My name is Randy Gardner.
CONTESTANT #3 (Contestant, To Tell the Truth, File Footage, May 11, 1964): My name is Randy Gardner.
NARRATOR: The experiment won him first place in his high school science fair and caught the attention of a nation.
BRUCE MCALLISTER (Author): He ended up on To Tell the Truth, you know, a T.V. show. It was supposedly the most written about story in the world after J.F.K. and the Beatles.
PEGGY CASS (Panelist, To Tell the Truth, File Footage, May 11, 1964): Number 2, how did you pass the time when the other people were sleeping?
RANDY GARDNER (File Footage, May 11, 1964): Well, there were always, there was always someone with me. The two boys that helped me, one would sleep, and one would stay awake.
NARRATOR: Bruce McAllister was one of those boys.
BRUCE MCALLISTER: It wasn’t the science of it that interested the world. The world was interested in the drama.
NARRATOR: The experiment moved beyond high school science, when sleep researcher William Dement joined the team.
BRUCE MCALLISTER: And he brought a portable E.E.G., which no one had ever seen before. They sent the E.E.G.s to a supercomputer, a Cray computer, in Arizona, and that computer concluded that parts of his brain were sleeping while others were awake. His brain was catnapping in pieces. That is how the human brain survived this.
NARRATOR: While Randy looked wide awake, parts of his brain weren’t; what neuroscientists now call a “microsleep.”
YUVAL NIR: A microsleep is something that may happen after we’ve been awake for a very long time, and our brain needs sleep so desperately that we may fall for a very short interval of three to 15 seconds just…and we’ve all seen this when our eyes shut down, and we nod to sleep for just a few seconds like this.
DAVID DINGES: We’re all very polite and, sort of, pretend we don’t see it, but as we’re talking to someone, they start to fall asleep, we notice that they’re losing muscle tone, so they start to slump, and the head will start to fall over and the eye, the lids are coming down, and then we’ll see the eyes roll in the head. If you’re holding a steering wheel and driving, you might notice that your arms are starting to slack a little bit on the wheel.
ETI BEN SIMON: It only takes 200 milliseconds, when you’re not paying attention, that the car is going in the in the wrong direction.
NARRATOR: When you’re sleep deprived, your brain will fall asleep, whether you notice or not. That’s because biology takes the wheel. Two different processes drive you to sleep; the first is your circadian rhythm, your biological clock.
RAVI ALLADA: One of the signals that’s keeping you awake during the day is the circadian clock. Think of the circadian clock as kind of an internal alarm clock.
NARRATOR: Back in 1938, in a landmark experiment, sleep researcher Nathaniel Kleitman takes one of his students to live in an underground cave. They spend a month without sunlight, testing the power of that internal alarm clock.
Even without light, the clock keeps ticking, but add light and the cycle can shift.
PHYLLIS ZEE: Every day, you get a little light in the morning, it moves your clock in one direction; a little light in the evening, it moves your clock in the other direction. So, it can delay and advance, and by doing this, it maintains your internal clock in synchrony with that of your external environment.
NARRATOR: This clock controls the release of a key chemical that has the power to make you feel sleepy: melatonin, also known as the hormone of darkness.
PHYLLIS ZEE: Melatonin goes up at night and stays up during the entire night, until probably the early morning hours, and then begins its decline. By the time that we normally would be waking up, melatonin levels are very, very low.
NARRATOR: But sleepiness isn’t just controlled by melatonin. Another chemical, called adenosine, also may play a role. Some researchers believe it starts to build from the moment you wake up and, like an hourglass, fills with the passage of time, gradually increasing our need for sleep, called “sleep pressure.”
ETI BEN SIMON: We see a very strong association between the levels of adenosine and being sleepy, and between the level of sleep and reducing that adenosine. Sleep is the perfect way to clear adenosine from the brain and start fresh.
PHILIP GEHRMAN: Many of us are trying to block the effects of that adenosine every day by our consumption of caffeine. Caffeine binds to these adenosine receptors, so it, kind of, blocks the effects of sleep pressure, so that we’re not feeling it. It’s still there, but we’re just not feeling the effects of it.
NARRATOR: But drinking coffee will work for just so long.
KETEMA PAUL: If you choose not to go to sleep tonight, and you stay up all night, that sleep pressure will continue to build. And as that sleep pressure builds, it will impair your thought processes. It will impair your memory.
PHYLLIS ZEE: More than 60 percent of our population may not be getting sufficient amounts of sleep but also sufficient quality of sleep.
NARRATOR: For firefighter Matt Reinhold, who often works a 72-hour shift, a good night’s sleep is hard to come by.
MATT REINHOLD: The alarm will go off and the lights will come on. It’s a, it’s a startlement, you know, right off the bat, you’re woken up. There’s times where we’ll run five, six, seven, eight calls after midnight. I have never been in a deep sleep here at this firehouse. Fatigue starts to set in. All I want to go and do is lay down, but then the alarm’s going off again for a medical down the street.
Lack of sleep definitely makes you more snippy. You become more agitated, more irritated. The communication factor may go out the window, because you don’t want to talk, because you’re tired, because of what you’re afraid of what you might say. The irritability takes a toll.
ETI BEN SIMON: There was a study that compared a group of participants only allowed to sleep for five hours a night, and then there was another group that were allowed to sleep for the whole eight hours but were woken up every now and then and kept awake for an hour. And after four days like this, the effects on mood and anxiety were actually much stronger in the interrupted group.
NARRATOR: Sleeping in fits and starts wreaks havoc on your entire system.
DAVID DINGES: That fragmenting of sleep is extremely destructive for wake functioning and health. It is almost as though you didn’t get sleep. So, the consolidation of sleep is easily as important as the duration of the sleep.
NARRATOR: Quality is as important as quantity.
KETEMA PAUL: There’s something about the loss of sleep that breaks down your mind and your body. If you lose sleep, you will experience memory deficits, and you will experience cognitive deficits. However, there’s also an interesting caveat to that; losing sleep may affect one person one way and another person another way. There are some individuals which tend to be resilient against the negative effects of sleep loss. One of the current focuses of our field is to understand the cause of that resilience and whether there is a genetic component.
NARRATOR: Could the ability to recover from sleep loss, be determined by our genes?
In 2017, the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine was awarded to three scientists who discovered a group of genes that drive your biological clock.
KETEMA PAUL: There are genes such as the clock gene, the period gene, the cryptochrome gene.
NARRATOR: But the gene that intrigues neuroscientist Ketema Paul is called BMAL1.
KETEMA PAUL: For those of you that know that clocks used to have gears, BMAL1 is the primary gear of the clock.
NARRATOR: In his lab at U.C.L.A., Ketema is exploring the role BMAL1 may play in our ability to recover from sleep loss. To do it, he makes use of a classic memory test.
KETEMA PAUL: You put a mouse in an environment and present two objects.
NARRATOR: In this case two orange blocks. The mouse spends time getting familiar with them.
KETEMA PAUL: After that, we take them out, and we sleep deprive them for six hours.
NARRATOR: While the mouse is kept awake, one of the orange blocks is replaced with a new object, a blue cylinder. After six hours, the sleep-deprived mouse is put back in the chamber.
Like humans, mice are naturally curious, they’re drawn to new things. So a well-rested mouse will spend more time exploring the new object, the blue cylinder. But that doesn’t happen with this sleepy mouse.
KETEMA PAUL: If you sleep deprive a mouse, and you put it in the same environment, it will spend equal amounts of time between the familiar object and the novel object, because its memory of the familiar object was impaired by the sleep loss.
NARRATOR: It seems not to remember the orange block. But what happens if Ketema repeats the memory test with a mouse that has been genetically modified to express higher levels of BMAL1? Will this dialed up mouse have a better memory? Or will the results be the same?
KETEMA PAUL: The mouse is now exploring the area in which the novel object is in: another approach to the novel object, more exploration around the arena, a third approach to the novel object, sniffing, exploring the novel object. So, the mouse is clearly spending more time with the novel object, and it suggests that the sleep deprivation did not impair the memory of this mouse, which suggests that overexpressing BMAL1 does make that mouse resilient to sleep loss, and it preserves its memory after sleep deprivation.
NARRATOR: But this is not the only surprise, because it turns out BMAL1 works in mysterious ways.
KETEMA PAUL: So the B in BMAL1 stands for “brain.” M in BMAL1 stands for “muscle.”
NARRATOR: BMAL1 is a gene that’s found in the brain and in skeletal muscle. And the resilient mice were ones that had BMAL1 boosted in their muscles, not in their brains.
KETEMA PAUL: The result we got was really a surprise. Sleep is a mental process, and that’s, kind of, as a trained neurobiologist, how I had been looking at it before that.
NARRATOR: As Ketema continues his research, he will try to find out how genes in skeletal muscle could influence how we sleep and store memories.
KETEMA PAUL: Hopefully, this will lead to more effective therapies for people that are unable to get sufficient sleep and people that need to function in spite of not getting enough sleep.
NARRATOR: Because when it comes to not getting enough sleep, the people hardest hit are often the ones we depend on the most.
SUSAN FARREN (First Responders Resiliency, Inc.): Sleep deprivation of first responders leads to issues of increased isolation and depression and has enormous impacts, not on just your psychology, but on your physical wellbeing, as well.
NARRATOR: Former E.M.S. worker Susan Farren and retired firefighter Ron Shull are working with first responders, educating them on ways to manage their sleep, on and off the job.
RON SHULL (First Responders Resiliency, Inc.): I want to give you guys some tools and some techniques and modalities to kind of help you sleep better.
YUVAL NIR: In the last 10 or 20 years, we’ve discovered that sleep is essential for proper brain function. It’s important for our health, for our immunity, for our memory and for our wellbeing.
REBECCA SPENCER: I think that’s where the phrase “sleep on it” comes from, is that we wake up and we think differently about a problem. We think differently about that person that we were angry at. We just generally feel better about our day, if we’ve had a really good night’s sleep.
DAVID DINGES: I think sleep’s one of the sweetest things you can get. It’s like a great meal or seeing a good friend.
KETEMA PAUL: As a citizen in our society that wants people to be safe, the best thing I can say is don’t sacrifice your sleep. Protect your sleep like you protect your food, like you protect your resources, like you protect your environment. It may save your life.
EDITED BY
Jedd Ehrmann
WRITTEN, PRODUCED AND DIRECTED BY
Terri Randall
NARRATED BY
Jay O. Sanders
CAMERA
David Linstrom
Vincente Franco
Thomas van Krugten
Sidney Lubitsch
Jason Longo
Jeff Saunders
Phil Bradshaw
Max Branigan
Natalia Bougadellis
ANIMATION
Brian Edgerton
MUSIC
Chee Wei Tay
ASSOCIATE PRODUCER
Avner Tavori
RESEARCH
Leora Randall-Tavori
SOUND RECORDISTS
Michael D. Guske
Loretta Molitor
Keith Sikora
W.L.J.M de Wijs
Andy Turrett
Jim Keen
J. Scott Buckley
Andy Wenrich
ASSISTANT CAMERA
Nadir Soltan
Amasha Lyons-Clark
ONLINE EDITING
Goldcrest Post Production
COLORIST
Evan Anthony
AUDIO MIX
Heart Punch Studio
LOCATION ASSISTANCE
Aquarium of the Pacific
Phoenix Enterprise
ARCHIVAL MATERIAL
Ann Froschauer, USFWS
Division of Rare and Manuscript Collections, Cornell University Library
Defense Visual Information Distribution Service
Elia Nazarenko
Frederick W. Kent Collection, The University of Iowa Libraries, Iowa City, Iowa
Getty Images
Itazawa Shijima
Lytx
Pond5
Rick Wassing
San Diego Historical Society
Shutterstock
The Scene - Eus Van Someren, Marijke Klasema
University of Chicago Photographic Archive
American Journal of Psychology, Board of Trustees of the University of Illinois and the University of Illinois Press
SPECIAL THANKS
California Department of Forestry and Fire Protection
City of Long Beach, CA
City of Perris, CA
Hennepin Healthcare Sleep Center
Northwestern Memorial Hospital
Spinoza Center for Neuroimaging
Amarillis Arroyo
Betthany Jones
Carlos H. Schenck
Edward Appel
Maha Ashraf
Marybeth O’Connor
Mathias Basner
Megan Larson
Michael Scullin
Natalie Bryant
Payton Andrews
Prof. Robert Proctor
Rosemary Braun
Sanna Lokhandwala
Stephen George
NOVA SERIES GRAPHICS
yU + co.
NOVA THEME MUSIC
Walter Werzowa
John Luker
Musikvergnuegen, Inc.
ADDITIONAL NOVA THEME MUSIC
Ray Loring
Rob Morsberger
CLOSED CAPTIONING
The Caption Center
POST PRODUCTION ONLINE EDITOR
Lindsey Rundell Denault
-----
SENIOR DIGITAL PRODUCER
Ari Daniel
DIGITAL MANAGING PRODUCER
Kristine Allington
DIGITAL PRODUCER
Emily Zendt
DIGITAL ASSOCIATE PRODUCERS
Ana Aceves
Arlo Pérez
DIGITAL PRODUCTION ASSISTANT
Lorena Lyon
-----
DIRECTOR OF EDUCATION AND OUTREACH
Ralph Bouquet
OUTREACH MANAGER
Gina Varamo
PROGRAM MANAGER, NOVA SCIENCE STUDIO
Tenijah Hamilton
-------
DIRECTOR OF AUDIENCE DEVELOPMENT
Dante Graves
DIRECTOR OF PUBLIC RELATIONS
Jennifer Welsh
AUDIENCE ENGAGEMENT EDITOR
Sukee Bennett
NATIONAL PUBLICITY
DKC Public Relations
DIRECTOR OF NATIONAL AUDIENCE RESEARCH
Cory Allen
-------
ASSOCIATE RESEARCHER
Christina Monnen
PRODUCTION COORDINATOR
Linda Callahan
PRODUCTION ASSISTANT
Angelica Coleman
-------
LEGAL AND BUSINESS AFFAIRS
Susan Rosen
BUSINESS MANAGER
Elisabeth Frele
RIGHTS MANAGER
Hannah Gotwals
PARALEGAL
Sarah Erlandson
TALENT RELATIONS
Janice Flood
-------
BROADCAST MANAGER
Nathan Gunner
SUPERVISING PRODUCER
Kevin Young
SENIOR PROMOTIONS PRODUCER AND EDITOR
Michael H. Amundson
POST PRODUCTION ASSOCIATE PRODUCER
Jay Colamaria
----
SENIOR EXECUTIVE PRODUCER, EMERITA
Paula S. Apsell
----
SENIOR SCIENCE EDITOR
Evan Hadingham
COORDINATING PRODUCER
Elizabeth Benjes
PROJECT DIRECTOR
Pamela Rosenstein
DEVELOPMENT PRODUCER
David Condon
NOVA PRODUCER
Caitlin Saks
SCIENCE EDITOR
Robin Kazmier
SENIOR SERIES PRODUCER
Melanie Wallace
DIRECTOR, BUSINESS OPERATIONS & FINANCE
Laurie Cahalane
EXECUTIVE PRODUCERS
Julia Cort Chris Schmidt
A NOVA production by Terri Randall Productions for WGBH Boston
© 2020 WGBH Educational Foundation
All rights reserved
This program was produced by WGBH, which is solely responsible for its content. Some funders of NOVA also fund basic science research. Experts featured in this film may have received support from funders of this program.
Original funding for this program was provided by Draper, the David H. Koch Fund for Science, the NOVA Science Trust, and the Corporation for Public Broadcasting.
IMAGE:
Image credit: (woman sleeping)
© Tiffany Bryant/Shutterstock
- Drew Ackerman, Ravi Allada, Eti Ben-Simon, Michel Cramer Bornemann, David Dinges, Susan Farren, Philip Gehrman, Rebecca Gomez, Bruce McAllister, Reiny Metz, Yuval Nir, Ketema Paul, Gina Poe, Matthew Reinhold, Jerry Siegel, Rebecca Spencer, Eus Van Someren, Matthew Walker, Phyllis Zee
Explore More

Poor-quality sleep could prime the brain for an anxious day

Mysteries of Sleep

How Sleep Can Heal Our Brains

How Sleep Helps Children Learn and Remember New Words

Can Sleep Quality and Quantity be Boosted Using Sound?

Blue Computer Light Alone May Not Be Messing With Your Sleep

The Nightmare of Sleep Paralysis

Scientists Track “Social Jet Lag” Sleeping Habits With Twitter Data
