Background and Introduction |
Camper Survey Tests and Experiments |
Sample Graphs of Survey Results |
Compare your results with campers from the A Science Odyssey Camp-In
Camp-In Curriculum: Putting It Together -- Camper Survey
Background and Introduction
Pulling together pieces from the previous activities, campers will conduct a
survey about themselves, organize their results, and communicate the data with
each other. Surveys are used all the time to get a clearer image of a large
amount of information. Computer databases are used to organize information and
search for patterns. Telecommunications allows distant researchers to
communicate and pool their data.
In this survey, we want to know more about the people in our group. People
will be asked to observe a variety of traits and do some experiments, creating
a database with their responses. Some of the traits are genetically inherited.
Some traits are not considered genetic, but may change as people grow older or
vary by where people live.
Before compiling and sharing the results, make some predictions. For example:
Do more people have attached earlobes or unattached ear lobes? In the entire
group, how many people do you think will have whorled fingerprints? Make a
graph to see if any patterns emerge. Is hand dominance related to eye
dominance? Are more boys sensitive to PTC taste than girls? Some sample graphs
are shown at the bottom of this page (Sample Graphs of Survey Results).
Youngsters are interested in knowing about others who share their traits. How
many members of the group were born in August? Share the results once they are
compiled. Feel free to add or omit questions or adapt the survey to your
needs.
Details, procedures, and materials for answering each specific question follow
the survey questions.
Camper Survey Printout
Compare your results with campers from the A Science Odyssey Camp-In
back to the top
Camper Survey Tests and Experiments
Experiment: Hand Dominance
Materials
paper
pencil
beans (such as dried kidney or navy beans)
spoons
ball
Background for Instructors
Are you right handed or left handed? This seemingly simple question can
be an interesting exercise in designing a scientific experiment. How do you
decide if someone is left handed or right handed? You can ask people, but how
do you know the answers are accurate? Encourage everyone to think
scientifically and to learn more by experimenting and collecting evidence.
Procedure
- Develop and try tests that might determine hand dominance. For
example:
1. Write your name using your right hand and then your left hand. Compare the
writing. Is one "better" than the other? How do you define "better?" Is this a
fair test?
2. Have someone scoop up some beans with a spoon using the right hand and then
the left. Observe with which hand the person is better able complete the
task.
3. Toss a ball (carefully) and observe which hand the person uses to catch
it.
- Does each test yield the same results? Are there people who use both
hands (ambidextrous)? How do the test results compare to people's
expectations?
Experiment: Eye Dominance
Materials
index card with a small hole in the center

Background for Instructors
Right / left dominance is the tendency to favor one side of the body
over the other. People generally have a dominant eye, meaning the brain chooses
between images perceived by the right eye and the left eye. If neither eye is
dominant, the image jumps from one eye to the other. There is no accepted
correlation between hand dominance and eye dominance.
Procedure
- Hold an index card with a small hole in the center at arm's
length. Look through the hole with both eyes at a distant object. Close one
eye, then the other. The eye that keeps the object centered in the hole is the
dominant eye.
Experiment: Tongue Rolling

Materials
none
Background for Instructors
The ability for tongue rolling is generally accepted to be an inherited
trait and can be tested easily by observation (and goofiness).
Procedure
- Ask each camper to try to roll the tip of his tongue into a
circle.
- Try other tongue "tricks." Can people flip their tongues upside down?
Which way do people flip their tongues-to the left, right, or both? Is tongue
flipping related to tongue rolling? (Can non-tongue rollers flip their tongue?)
Can anyone touch the tip of his nose with his tongue?
Experiment: Ear Lobes

Materials
none
Background for Instructors
Ear lobe attachment is generally accepted to be an inherited trait and
can be tested by observation.
Procedure
- Observe whether the bottom of a person's ear lobes are attached or
unattached.
- Observe and discuss the variation in this trait. Is it always clear
whether someone has attached ear lobes? Does anyone have partially attached ear
lobes?
Experiment: PTC Taste Test
Materials
PTC (phenylthiocarbamide) paper
control paper
mints (optional)
Background for Instructors
Sensitivity to the taste of the chemical PTC is inherited.
Approximately 70% of the population senses a strong bitter taste while other
people taste nothing.
Procedure
- Work in pairs. One person is the experimenter, the other is the
subject. The experimenter takes one piece of PTC paper and one piece of control
paper (no taste).
- Ask the subject to taste one of the papers and record the response. Then
offer the second paper and record the response. Ask the subject to compare the
two tastes. Is the subject sensitive to PTC?
- Reverse roles -- the subject becomes the experimenter and repeats the
test.
- Some people do experience a strong bitter taste, and may appreciate a
strong, pleasant flavor such as a mint to counteract the bitter taste.
Experiment: Fingerprinting


Materials
fingerprint patterns (included)
soft pencils
paper
clear tape (wide "magic" tape works best)
hand lens
soapy water and paper towels (for washing up)
Background for Instructors
The tips of a person's fingers have small friction ridges on them.
Along the ridges are small pores which secrete salt, water, and proteins. These
substances along with oil that your fingers may pick up from your skin or hair
will be deposited on things that you touch, leaving fingerprints. Your
overall fingerprints are unique to you. As you grow, your fingerprints grow
bigger but the pattern is always the same. Scientists classify fingerprints
into several general patterns. The following is an easy procedure for making
clear fingerprints to examine. You may want to involve volunteers from local
law enforcement agencies in fingerprinting activities.
Procedure
- With a soft pencil make a dark spot a little bigger than a
fingertip. Fill it in completely and go over it many times.
- Prepare a piece of clear tape about 5 cm. (2 in.) long. Try to only touch
the very ends of the tape, and hang it from the edge of a table or shelf.
- Press a finger gently in the darkened pencil spot and roll it slightly
from side to side.
- Place the tape over the end of the finger with the pencil graphite on it
and then carefully place the tape on the blank fingerprint template card.
- Try to identify the fingerprint pattern by matching it to the samples on
the chart.
- Repeat the steps for the other fingers. Campers may need to go over the
pencil spot.
Extension: Who dunit?
- Divide into small groups of 5 to 10 campers.
- Everyone in the group makes one fingerprint on a small card and writes
her initials on the back.
- Collect all the cards, mix them up, and put them in an envelope or cup.
- Everyone makes a duplicate, recording the exact same fingerprint, (and
same initials) on a second card.
- Lay out the second set of fingerprints for everyone to see.
- Pull one of the fingerprint cards from the first set and see if you can
match it to one from the second set, without looking at the initials on the
back.
Fingerprint Patterns Printout
Experiment: Reaction Time
Materials
ruler with centimeters marked
calculators (optional, for averaging results)
Background for Instructors
When you see a movement or hear a sound, your eyes and ears send
electrical signals along nerve pathways to the visual and auditory centers of
your brain. These centers then relay messages telling your muscles to react.
What causes reaction time to change? Do you get faster with practice? It could
be interesting to correlate reaction time with participants' age. Do children
react faster or slower than adults?
Though it is difficult, try to make this a non-competitive activity.
This can be a good opportunity to talk about variables and controls. How can
you be sure everyone is starting from the same point? What if one person jerks
her hand as she drops the ruler? How can you make this a "fair"
experiment?
Procedure
- Work in pairs.
- One partner holds a ruler vertically in an outstretched hand. The zero
mark should be towards the ground.
- The other person faces his partner and holds his hand outstretched at the
very bottom of the ruler at the zero mark, but not touching it.
- Without warning, the person holding the ruler releases it. Her partner
tries to react quickly and grab the ruler. The centimeter mark at the place
where the ruler is caught will be our measure of reaction time. The higher the
centimeter mark the farther the ruler fell and the slower the person's reaction
time.
- Each person should repeat the experiment three times and take an average
of his results.
Extensions
- Repeat the experiment but this time say "go" when you drop the
ruler. Does the reaction time change with this cue?
- Ask campers to close their eyes and try to catch the ruler using only a
sound cue.
back to the top
Sample Graphs of Survey Results
Here are a few sample graphs. You may choose to compile camper survey results
in other ways to show what patterns and differences emerge.
back to the top
Home | Resources for Educators Menu | Camp-In Curriculum Contents | Help
WGBH | PBS Online | Search | Feedback | Shop
© 1998 WGBH
|